Asian Women - The Research Institute of Asian Women

Asian Women - Vol. 30, No. 4

Women Sales Personnel’s Emotion Management: Do Employee Affectivity, Job Autonomy, and Customer Incivility Affect Emotional Labor?

Gihyung Kim : Sookmyung Women’s University, South Korea
Seunghee Lee : Sookmyung Women’s University, South Korea

Journal Information
Journal ID (publisher-id): RIAW
Journal : Asian Women
ISSN: 1225-925X (Print)
Article Information
Print publication date: Month: 12 Year: 2014
Volume: 30 Issue: 4
First Page: 3 Last Page: 28
DOI: https://doi.org/10.14431/aw.2014.12.30.4.3

Abstract

The goal of this study is to examine the antecedents and outcomes of emotional labor from women sales personnel in the clothing industry. Despite the growing number of people working in the service sector, relatively little empirical research has examined the effects and outcomes of emotional labor with an integrated research model. Based on the previous literature, this study designed a research model analyzing structural equation modeling in which women sales personnel were faced with emotional labor. Data from 239 employees revealed that emotional labor was significantly affected by employee affectivity, job autonomy, and customer incivility. This study found that the surface acting affected by employee affectivity and customer incivility was significantly related to turnover intention and burnout. Also, the deep acting affected by job autonomy was significantly related to self-efficacy and turnover intention. Theoretical and practical implications of the findings and future directions are discussed.


Introduction

With a robust and steady growth in the service sector, service employees’ emotional labor has been a relevant topic of research inquiry. Employees conduct emotional labor by regulating expressions and feelings in interaction with customers, and consequently emotion regulation has become a crucial part of daily work. After Hochschild’s (1983) definition of emotional labor, numerous scholars have complemented conceptual and theoretical studies of emotional labor (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Brotheridge & Lee, 2003; Glomb & Tews, 2004; Grandey, 2000; Morris & Feldman, 1996; Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987). Moreover, empirical studies have found that emotional labor was directly associated with a range of consumer attitudes including perception of customer service quality (Silter et al., 2010; Totterdell & Holman, 2003), customer satisfaction (Otieno, Harrow, & Lea-Greenwood, 2005), and purchase intention (Tang et al., 2013), in numerous occupations such as flight attendants (Hochschild, 1983), convenience store clerks (Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987), and healthcare professionals (Miller, Stiff, & Ellis, 1988).

Clothing stores are prime examples of places to investigate the concept of emotional labor because service employees are typically characterized by direct and intimate interaction with customers on the spot (Godwyn, 2006). In addition, the participation rate of women is much higher in occupation groups since skills that involve handling the distinct characteristics of clothing items and empathizing with customer needs and tastes are required. Stress literature found that women are much more sensitive and vulnerable to emotional events that are significantly related to job stress (Burk, 2002). We consider that women employees in the clothing service sector are an adequate research group to investigate attributes of emotional labor and its corresponding relationship.

Despite these findings, relatively little empirical research has examined the integrated relationship among the antecedents and outcomes of emotional labor. This study focuses on the following constraints and limitations. First, the occurrence of emotional labor has been investigated frequently, but the reasons that lead to the emotional labor have been less defined. Multi-levels of antecedent variables should be empirically examined, especially customers’ uncivil behavior as a newly focused concept impacting emotional labor (Silter et al., 2010; Spencer & Rupp, 2009). Second, employees’ perspectives on emotional labor have been relatively less studied than those of customers. As a main agent of service work, an employee’s specific experiences and feelings are a key element when discussing emotional labor. Last, most literature focused only on the negative effects of emotional labor, but positive effects should be examined simultaneously as an outcome of emotional labor.

Therefore, this study considered the reasons for emotional labor in terms of perspectives from the employee, organization, and customers, and extended the result of the emotional labor to be positive or negative. By integrating these results with the theories of emotional labor, we developed a research model for an emotional labor process and tested the hypotheses with empirical data. This study explores deeper insights in functioning emotional labor research and further establishes effective emotional management strategies for women employees in the clothing service sector.


Literature Review
Emotional labor

While focusing on the difference between actual feeling and the feeling of expression by actors in dramaturgy (Goffman, 1959), Hochschild (1979, 1983) adjusted the concept of emotional labor into organizational culture and the original definition as “the management of feeling to create a publicly observable facial and bodily display.” After Hochschild’s study, several conceptualizations of emotional labor were proposed (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Brotheridge & Lee, 2003; Glomb & Tews, 2004; Grandey, 2000; Morris & Feldman, 1996; Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987). Emotional labor was an exchange value, a wage for “the act of displaying appropriate emotion” (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993) following the display rules for the organizational goals (Grandey, 2000).

Ashforth and Humphrey (1993) asserted the appropriateness of display rules rather than feeling rules (Hochschild, 1979, 1983) because rules for emotional labor refer to a set of certain behaviors rather than internal feelings. According to Ekman (1972), facial expressions for specific emotions that a person experiences and particular facial configurations are connected universally. For optimal control of the service quality, emotional labor has to be easily monitored by managers, co-workers, and customers (Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987). Mastracci, Newman, and Guy (2010) classify emotional labor as pretense or authentic display by cognitive skills. In emotive work, employees conform to the display rule and regulate the expression of emotion. For instance, display rule for sales personnel in fashion retail stores is to express positive emotion (happiness, passion) and to suppress negative emotion (anxiety, irritation).

Emotional labor has been conceptualized in two ways. First, job-focused emotional labor is the behavioral response to variations in the duration, frequency, intensity, variety, and emotional dissonance of service interactions (Morris & Feldman, 1996), which means “the level of emotional demands” in an occupation (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002). However, other research has found that emotional labor does not always involve or lead to emotional dissonance (Brotheridge & Lee, 2003; Morris & Feldman, 1997; Zerbe, 2000). Emotional dissonance is a state of being rather than an objective or a process (Grandey, 2000), so it is difficult to say what connects it to the emotion management process. The relationship between emotional dissonance and emotional labor is more likely to include conditional circumstances (Christensen & Raynor, 2003; Godwyn, 2006; Gutek, 1995). Because of ambiguity and controversy in these debates, we do not consider emotional dissonance as a component of emotional labor in our research model.

Second, employee-focused emotional labor describes “the process of managing emotion and expression” by employees to meet work demands (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002) while performing through surface acting and deep acting (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Hochschild, 1979, 1983). Groth, Hennig-Thurau, and Walsh (2009) defined surface acting as “faking or amplifying emotions by displaying emotions not actually felt,” and deep acting as “attempting to modify felt emotions so that a genuine emotional display follows.” Surface acting involves employees simply pretending to fulfill their job duty, which is achieved by modulating their reaction to a situation such as faking a smile or suppressing a bad mood (Totterdell & Holman, 2003). Deep acting, in contrast, involves employees trying to align required and true feelings (Hülsheger & Schewe, 2011), which is achieved by modifying their arousal or perception of a situation. Since this study is highly relevant to employees’ perceptions and experiences along with performing emotional labor, we consider surface acting and deep acting to be components of emotional labor in our formulation.

Antecedents in influencing emotional labor

Affectivity defined as “a general tendency to experience a particular mood or to react to objects in a particular way or with certain emotion” (Lazarus, 1991). In emotional labor, affectivity is an important employee characteristic that is associated with emotional labor and directly influences employee mood states on the job (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Brotheridge & Lee, 2003). As the sum of an individual’s affective disposition, the affect-based personality trait explores the relationship with an emotional labor strategy (Cheung & Tang, 2009).

A person with negative affectivity, or neuroticism, may have a predisposition to view the world in negative terms, such as pessimism and aversion, and assign a more negative interpretation to stressful work events (Abraham, 1999). On the contrary, a person with positive affectivity, or extroversion, may have a positive disposition including enthusiasm, involvement, and commitment (Watson, Pennebaker, & Folger, 1987). Gosserand and Diefendorff (2005) found negative affectivity was positively related to surface acting, whereas positive affectivity was positively related to deep acting. In this study, we considered affectivity as one of the antecedent variables in influencing emotional labor.

Job autonomy is a widely studied work characteristic, which reflects freedom, independence, and discretion in work scheduling, decision- making, and work methods (Hackman & Oldham, 1976; Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006). In emotional labor, autonomy is a broad concept that can generate specific emotions and behaviors as an antecedent (Grandey, 2000; Gross, 1998; Morris & Feldman, 1996) or can mediate the relationship between emotional labor and individual well-being (Johnson & Spector, 2007).

Employees with a low level of job autonomy are more likely to follow the organization’s display rule strictly and to experience emotional exhaustion as a consequence of surface acting (Grandey, Fisk, & Steiner, 2005). Meanwhile, employees with high level of job autonomy recognize surface acting as less demanding (Johnson & Spector, 2007) and comfortably choose it as one of their selling methods. In this study, we consider that autonomy may alleviate emotional dissonance and emotional exhaustion for service workers in jobs with a high level of emotional labor (Johanson & Woods, 2008). According to the theoretical framework (Grandey, 2000; Gross, 1998; Morris & Feldman, 1996), we investigate autonomy as one of the variables in influencing emotional labor, rather than as a moderator.

Customer incivility is defined as “low-intensity deviant behavior, perpetrated by someone in a customer or client role, with ambiguous intent to harm an employee, in violation of social norms of mutual respect and courtesy” (Silter et al., 2010). While the previously suggested concept of workplace incivility (Andersson & Pearson, 1999) is investigated in the perspective of employee-to-employee or supervisor-to-employee interaction, customer incivility is a norm newly discussed as employee- to-customer interaction.

The literature found sales- and service-related employees experience incivility more often from customers than co-workers and confront with higher burnout levels (Grandey, Kern, & Frone, 2007). When the employees are faced with customers’ disrespectful treatment at work, they endure the mistreatment and convey courteous service quality (Ben-Zur & Yagil, 2005). Customer incivility is an important point to affect emotional labor in sales and service sector since customer are not always right (Grandey, Dickter, & Sin, 2004). With the rising interest in incivility from customers, we consider it to be an important variable in influencing emotional labor.

Emotional labor and outcomes

In the literature, the effects of psychological outcomes from emotional labor were typically related to negative norms such as stress, depression, cynicism, job dissatisfaction, turnover intention, emotional exhaustion and deviation, drug and alcohol abuse, and absenteeism (Grandey, 2000; Hochschild, 1979, 1983; Morris & Feldman, 1996).

The implementation of emotional labor impairs the “real self,” then raises “self-alienation,” and extends to emotional exhaustion (Hochschild, 1979). Consequently, the severe emotional exhaustion in emotional labor leads to burnout, which includes emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and diminished personal accomplishment (Maslach & Jackson, 1981). Turnover intention is a willingness to leave the organization as a negative outcome of emotional labor (Tett & Meyer, 1993), which is positively affected by surface acting (Chau et al., 2009). Most empirical studies have examined emotional dissonance as a mediating effect rather than a direct relationship between emotional labor and a negative outcome. We believe that those outcomes will be positively affected by surface acting and negatively affected by deep acting.

Although the negative effects came to the forefront, several scholars have found the positive effects of emotional labor (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987). Based on the “facial feedback hypothesis,” Adelmann (1995) proposed that a positive emotional expression, regardless of actual feeling- enhanced self-efficacy, led to job satisfaction, self-esteem, and then individual and organizational well-being. That is to say, expressing positive emotion leads to feeling the actual positive emotion.

Self-efficacy is the belief that one can successfully accomplish task requirements; it is related to the positive outcome of emotional labor (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993). Deep acting consolidates the self-efficacy by fulfilling social expectation, and voids embarrassing interpersonal problems by contributing to the formation of latent defense mechanisms (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002). In clothing retail stores, sales associates at the beginner level are encouraged to perform surface acting intentionally, which protects genuine emotion from incivility and dissonance, and enhances task performance and self-efficacy (Lee & Kim, 2012). We believe that self-efficacy will be facilitated by the positive outcomes of emotional labor.


Objectives of the Study

This study examined the influence of antecedent variables from personal, organizational, and customers’ perspectives of emotional labor to investigate the impact that surface acting and deep acting have on individual well-being. Based on the theoretical literature previously presented, the following hypotheses were formulated.

  • H1. Negative affectivity will be positively related to surface acting (1a), but negatively related to deep acting (1b).
  • H2. A perceived high level of job autonomy will be negatively related to surface acting (2a), but positively related to deep acting (2b).
  • H3. Perceived customer incivility will be positively related to surface acting (3a), but negatively related to deep acting (3b).
  • H4. Self-efficacy will be negatively affected by surface acting (4a), but positively affected by deep acting (4b).
  • H5. Turnover intention will be negatively affected by surface acting (5a), but positively affected by deep acting (5b).
  • H6. Burnout will be positively affected by surface acting (6a), but negatively affected by deep acting (6b).

The research model, based on the six hypotheses, is depicted in Figure 1. As the antecedents, employee affectivity and customer incivility positively affect to surface acting but negatively to deep acting (Liu et al., 2004; Silter et al., 2010). When job autonomy is high, surface acting will be diminished but deep acting will be reinforced (Diefendorff & Gosserand, 2003; Grandey et al., 2005). In emotional labor, surface acting will negatively affect self-efficacy but positively affect turnover intention and burnout (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Lewig & Dollard, 2003; Mikolajczak, Menil, & Luminet, 2007). On the other hand, deep acting will be the opposite and weak relationship with the consequences of emotional labor rather than surface acting (Bakker & Heuven, 2006).


Figure 1. 
Research model for the hypothesized relationship between research variables. Letters and numbers near arrows represent the research hypotheses.


Methodology and Sample Design
Sample

Caring job include emotive characteristics such as nurturance, empathy, self-sacrifice and kindness have long been regarded as women’s job. Since women are more sensible to emotional events and typically associated with emotive labor, our study selects women sales personnel in clothing store as subjects of research.

Table 1 provides an overview of final sample characteristics. A total of 245 women sales personnel in the clothing industry voluntarily participated in the study. Six inadequate samples were excluded, and a final of 239 samples were included for data analysis. The age group ranged from 20-59 with a relatively evenly distributed sample across the age brackets of 20-29 years (33.9%), 30-39 years (28.9%), and those above 40 years (37.2%), which means women in their 30s stopped working for some reason (e.g., childbirth and childrearing). Marital status was evenly distributed (unmarried = 51.5%, married = 48.5%). Participants had an average of 13.64 years of education, most stopping after a high-school diploma (41.0%). Other information collected included years employed in the service industry and position at the job.

Table 1. 
Demographic characterstics of survey respondents
Variables (N=239) n %
Age Group
20-29 81 33.9%
30-39 69 28.9%
40+ 89 37.2%
Martial Status
Not married 123 51.5%
Married 116 48.5%
Educational Level
High school graduate 98 41.0%
Some college 86 36.0%
College graduate 55 23.0%
Monthly wage (unit KRW)
less than 2,000K 80 33.5%
2,000K ~ > 3,000K 106 44.4%
3,000K ~ > 4,000K 38 15.9%
4,000K and up 15 6.3%
Workplace
Small business 79 33.1%
Specialty retail store 76 31.8%
Department store 48 20.1%
Oulet 23 9.6%
Duty free 13 5.4%
Employment
Self employed 90 37.7%
Company (Full time) 104 43.5%
Company (Part time) 34 14.2%
Company (Incentive) 11 4.6%
Occupation
Manager 83 34.7%
Sales associate 81 33.9%
Cashier 54 22.6%
Coordinator 21 8.8%
* All respondents are female

Instruments

Critical variables for this study included three causes of emotional labor from personal, organizational, and customer perspectives: salesperson affectivity, job autonomy, and customer incivility as antecedent variables. In addition, surface acting and deep acting in emotional labor were considered independent variables. Self-efficacy, job satisfaction, and burnout were explored as dependent variables; their interrelationship was empirically assessed using structural equation modeling.

Emotional labor was measured with the original scale that Choo, Kim, and Jun (2010) based on the study of Brotheridge and Grandey (2002). The scale contains 10 items rated on a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = never to 5 = always). The items were composed of two independent, but related factors (Grandey, 2000). Five items measure surface acting (α = .86), which refer to false expressions of emotion, and five items measure deep acting (α = .82), which refer to genuine expressions of emotion. One example of the item is: “I pretend to have emotions that I don’t really have” (see the Appendix). A factor analysis revealed a three-factor structure, in accordance with the two dimensions described above. Since the confidence coefficient of more than .60 is enough to proceed with an exploratory study (Nunnally, 1978), the reliabilities of the subscales measuring surface acting and deep acting were high enough.

Affectivity was measured by an amended version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) developed by Watson, Clark, and Tellegen (1988). The scale contains five items, rated on a 5-point, Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = not at all to 5 = extremely. The internal consistency reliability estimate for this scale was α = .87 in the current sample. The question reads: “I feel scared about my life.” Six items measuring job autonomy were adopted from the studies by Morgeson and Humphrey (2006), and measured with a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .87. One sample item is: “The job provides me with significant autonomy in making decisions.” Customer incivility was assessed with the original scale developed by Wilson and Holmvall (2013). The scale consisted of 10 items, each measuring perceptions of uncivil behavior from customer with 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = never, 5 = more than three time per day). Crombach’s alpha for this scale was .88. One sample item is: “Customers continued to complain despite your effort to assist them.”

Self-efficacy was adopted from the study of self-efficiency by Chen, Gully, and Eden (2001). The scale contains five items, rated on a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The internal consistency reliability estimate for the scale was α = .77 in the current sample. The sample statement is: “On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.” Turnover intention was extracted from the study by Lee and Kim (2012) based on Muliawan, Green, and Robb (2009). Four items were measured with a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .84. One sample item is: “I like my job better than the average worker does.” Nine items for measuring burnout were adopted from the studies of emotional exhaustion and depersonalization by Maslach and Jackson (1981), and measured with a 5-point, Likert-type scale (1 = not at all, 5 = always). Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .90. The sample statement includes: “I feel emotionally drained from my work.”

Procedure

The questionnaire was designed to gain insight into women who work in sales service in clothing industry. The self-report questionnaire was written in Korean, and completion took an average of 15 minutes. To avoid geographic bias, we used convenience sampling from sales associate of small business, specialty retail store, department stores, outlet stores, and duty free shops in the Seoul and Gyeonggi area, which represent 45% of total population of Korea. Survey progress was done in cooperation with Korean Information Research during a period of two weeks in May 2014. Out of 300 questionnaires, 245 were returned (82% response rate) and 6 samples with inadequate answers were excluded. The final sample of our study consisted of 239 women.

Data analysis

All research questions were tested using statistical analysis: descriptive statistics, frequency analysis, correlation analysis, reliability analysis, exploratory factor analysis, and confirmatory factor analysis were completed using the Statistical Package for the Social Science (SPSS) version 20 (Sung, 2007) and the Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS) version 21 (Lee & Lim, 2009).

Findings of the Study

The Pearson correlations between the research variables are shown in Table 2. Negative affectivity was positively correlated to surface acting (r = .28, p < .01) and negatively correlated to deep acting (r = -.30, p < .01). Job autonomy was negatively correlated to surface acting (r = -.31, p < .01) and positively correlated to deep acting (r = .43, p < .01). Customer incivility was positively correlated to surface acting (r = .19, p < .01) and negatively correlated to deep acting (r = -.26, p < .01). Surface acting was positively correlated to turnover intention (r = .22, p < .01) and burnout (r = .38, p < .01), but not significantly correlated to self-efficacy. Deep acting was positively correlated to self-efficacy (r = .23, p < .01), but not significantly correlated to turnover intention and burnout. The table shows that H1, H2, and H3 were all confirmed in relation to the emotional labor, but H4, H5, and H6 were only partially confirmed. These results provided preliminary support for the hypothesized relationship.

Table 2. 
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1 Employee affectivity 15.25 3.88 1
2 Job autonomy 16.69 3.45 -.48** 1
3 Customer incivility 12.69 3.60 .27** -.30** 1
4 Surface acting 18.91 4.23 .28** -.31** .19** 1
5 Deep acting 25.22 6.68 -.30** .43** -.26** -.28** 1
6 Self efficacy 17.16 2.57 -.18** .25** -.11 .03 .23** 1
7 Turnover intention 11.43 3.33 .32** -.32** .26** .22** -.06 -.12 1
8 Burnout 24.78 6.43 .39** -.23** .28** .38** -.06 -.09 .47** 1
**p < .01

All measures were subjected to confirmatory factor analysis for issues of dimensionality, convergent, and discriminant validity. Since coefficient alpha underestimates the reliability of multidimensional measure, the two-step procedure suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) was examined for the effective use of coefficient alpha.

We started the confirmatory factor analysis of the antecedents (i.e., employee affectivity, job autonomy, and customer incivility). Results showed that the three-factor model was acceptable, χ² (df = 41, N = 239) = 80.749, p < .001, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .06, root mean residual (RMR) = .04, goodness of fit index (GFI) = .94, Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) = .96, and comparative fit index (CFI) = .97. All items were significantly loaded on their respective variables. Next, the confirmatory factor analysis of the dependents (i.e., surface acting, deep acting, self-efficacy, turnover intention, and burnout) was tested. Results showed that the five-factor model was acceptable, χ² (df = 46, N = 239) = 137.503, p < .001, RMSEA = .091, RMR = .05, GFI =.91, TLI =.89 and CFI =.923. Therefore, measures in this research model captured distinctive constructs.

We examined the structural equation model depicted in Figure 1 using AMOS version 21 (Lee & Lim, 2009). The results demonstrated the following fit statistics for an eight-factor model was acceptable, χ² (df = 83, N = 239) = 224.190, p < .001, RMSEA =. 09, RMR =.09, GFI =.90, AGFI =.84, TLI =.86 and CFI =.91. Although GFI were same to .09 and TLI were slightly below .09, adjusted goodness of fit (AGFI) was above .80, which indicates a good fit. Also, RMSEA and CFI were greater than a threshold value; therefore a high degree of validity as well as reliability for the research model was achieved.

Estimates of the path coefficients for the hypothesized model are presented in Figure 2. An examination of the paths in the research model revealed that hypothesis 1 was partially supported, with negative affectivity being positively related to surface acting (H1a, r = .17, p <.05), but unrelated to deep acting (H1b). Hypothesis 2 was partially supported with job autonomy being unrelated to surface acting (H2a), but positively related to deep acting (H2b, r = .37, p <.001). Perceived customer incivility was positively related to surface acting (H3a, r = .30, p <.001), but unrelated to deep acting (H3b). Self-efficacy was not affected by surface acting (H4a), but positively affected by deep acting (H4b, r = .11, p <.001). Hypothesis 5 was fully supported because turnover intention had positive path to surface acting (H5a, r = .47, p <.001) and negative path to deep acting (H5b, r = .20, p <.05). Hypothesis 6 was partially supported, with burnout being positively affected by surface acting (H6a, r = .72, p <.001), but not affected by deep acting (H6b).


Figure 2. 
Numbers near arrows represent standardized coefficients (*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.) Dashed lines represent nonsignificant paths.


Conclusion
General Conclusions

In this study, we examined the relationships between emotional labor and its antecedents and outcomes for women employees in the clothing service sector. We found that, through surface acting, emotional labor was positively affected by employee affectivity and customer incivility, which positively related to turnover intention and burnout. In addition, emotional labor was positively affected by job autonomy and positively related to self-efficacy through deep acting. This finding reconfirmed that the negative antecedents were expressed through surface acting leading to negative outcomes and the positive antecedents were expressed through deep acting leading to positive results in the existing theory (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Grandey, 2000; Hochschild, 1983; Morris & Feldman. 1996; Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987).

Meanwhile, our finding was that surface acting was not affected by job autonomy and did not correlate to self-efficacy. Also, deep acting was not affected by employee affectivity and customer incivility did not correlate to burnout. With the exception of deep acting, which was significantly related to turnover intention, surface acting did not offset the positive outcome and deep acting did not improve the negative outcome. In other words, the positive antecedents did not always improve surface acting and the negative antecedents did not always offset deep acting; the relationship is still not clear since several studies found no significance or opposite ways of relation among emotional labor concepts (Adelmann, 1995; C. Wong, P. Wong, & Law, 2007). Consequently, differences with the prevailing theory of emotional labor were also confirmed.

The study uses an integrative approach to analyze emotional labor among the antecedents and the outcomes, and newly count influence of emotional labor from customer side, customer incivility. Moreover, the result focuses especially on women employees in clothing service sector since women have been required to be kind and intimate, and have faced strong pressure to work in caring job and hospitality.

Theoretical and Practical Implications

With a growing number of people working in the service sector and experiencing emotional labor, it is increasingly important to examine the causes and outcomes of emotional labor, whether they are positive or negative outcomes. This study first investigated the integrated research model of emotional labor and examined it with empirical data analysis. As noted earlier, previous researchers have not examined both the antecedents and outcomes of emotional labor in one model, which could provide a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanism of emotional labor.

This study’s research reveals a more comprehensive understanding of interaction between the antecedents and the outcomes of emotional labor. The research model faithfully followed the framework of the initial conceptual study (Grandey, 2000), and replicated previous research results suggesting that emotional labor was related to self-efficacy (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002), turnover intention (Chau et al., 2009; Tett & Meyer, 1993). Additionally, the study extended the antecedents to customer perspective, included employee and organization perspective, and examined the research model with a homogenous sample on empirical data analysis. Moreover, emotional dissonance that had been ambiguous in nature as a situational concept is difficult to discuss due to the impact of casual relations with emotional labor, so it is excluded from this research model (Christensen & Raynor, 2003; Godwyn, 2006; Zerbe, 2000). In the implementation of emotional labor, the respondents’ behavioral concepts, such as surface acting and deep acting, were based on the verification of final research model.

The specific target - women sales personnel in the clothing service industry - was a homogenous sample, which was more informative to researchers interested in the dynamics of contextual or organizational factors in fashion and clothing. Participants were asked to provide ratings at work in real time using experience sampling methodology (Beal & Weiss, 2003), which is easier when considering emotion-related constructs. Consequently, based on the more informative data, this study may contribute to identifying problems and suggesting practical solutions for women working in emotional labor.

A possible explanation of our findings is that employees that are more likely to have negative affectivity perform surface acting rather than deep acting as a result of experiencing turnover intention and strong burnout. Thus, corporate welfare programs and cultural support are recommended to offset the negative employee affectivity. Under the organizational culture, recognizing the job autonomy of service workers rather than restricting and reinforcing the display rule gives employees a stronger tendency to perform deep acting with sincerity rather than surface acting. Also, building a good interaction with customers through deep acting may improve employee self-efficacy. However, high job autonomy will not restrain surface acting of emotional labor, which may continue to be a unique selling technique in service sector. Employees who often face customer incivility and rudeness may perform surface acting rather than deep acting to defend themselves, and consequently may experience more severe negative outcomes such as turnover intention and burnout. The organization should clearly define its position vis a vis customers’ excessive insolence and accurately inform employees how to react in alignment with the company’s customer service policy. These findings may apply to advance retail environments in encouraging service workers who face emotional labor and to conduct in-depth surveys of business efficiency and confidence.

Study Limitations and Directions for Future Research

As with most research, this study has its limitations. First, although we measured eight factors in the integrated research model, other variables may account for the relationship between causes and outcomes with emotional labor and could be controlled in future research. Second, we focused the outcome on employee perspectives, which is a crucial element to change in emotional labor, but future research could examine the relationship between customer satisfaction and organizational well-being. Third, our study targets women workers in the clothing service sector, which may not be generalized for all types of jobs or other demographic factors such as sex and age. Even though most service workers in fashion industry are women, a comparative analysis of women and men with the variables would be crucial for future research since the relationship between the variables might be influenced by demographic factor. Fourth, although we have integrated antecedents and outcomes of emotional labor in one model, we are still not clear when emotional labor has a weaker or stronger effect on an individual’s psychological status. In other words, the moderating mechanism of the effects in emotional labor was not examined in our study. Last, the research model was based on a previous theory from initial studies of emotional labor, but future researchers could progress to a conceptual study, which could encompass the partial empirical study and the recent findings regarding emotional labor.


Acknowledgments

This Research was supported by the Sookmyung Women’s University Research Grants (1-1403-0152).


References
1. Abraham, R., (1999), The impact of emotional dissonance on organizational commitment and intention to turnover, Journal of Psychology, 133(4), p44-455. [https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989909599754]
2. Adelmann, P., (1995), Emotional labor as a potential source of job stress. In S. L. Sauter & L. Murphy (Eds.), Organizational risk factors for job stress, p371-381, Easton, MD: Easton Publishing. [https://doi.org/10.1037/10173-023]
3. Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W., (1988), Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two - step approach, Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), p411-423. [https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.103.3.411]
4. Andersson, L., & Pearson, C., (1999), Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace, Academy of Management Review, 24(3), p452-471. [https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.1999.2202131]
5. Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H., (1993), Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity, Academy of Management Reivew, 18(1), p88-115. [https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.1993.3997508]
6. Bakker, A. B., & Heuven, E., (2006), Emotional dissonance, burnout, and in-role performance among nurses and police officers, International Journal of Stress Management, 13(4), p423-440. [https://doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.13.4.423]
7. Beal, D. J., & Weiss, H. M., (2003), Methods of ecological momentary assessment in organizational research, Organizational Research Methods, 6(4), p440-464. [https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428103257361]
8. Ben-Zur, H., & Yagil, D., (2005), The relationship between empowerment, aggressive behaviors of customers, coping, and burnout, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14(1), p81-99. [https://doi.org/10.1080/13594320444000281]
9. Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A., (2002), Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives of “people work”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60(1), p17-39.
10. Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T., (2003), Development and validation of the emotional labour scale, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76(3), p365-379. [https://doi.org/10.1348/096317903769647229]
11. Burk, R. J., (2002), Work stress and women’s health: Occupational status effects, Journal of Business Ethics, 37(1), p91-102.
12. Chau, S. L., Dahling, J. J., Levy, P. E., & Diefendorff, J. M., (2009), A predictive study of emotional labor and turnover, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(8), p1151-1163. [https://doi.org/10.1002/job.617]
13. Chen, G., Gully, S. M., & Eden, D., (2001), Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale, Organizational Research Methods, 4(1), p62-83. [https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810141004]
14. Cheung, F. Y., & Tang, C. S., (2009), The influence of emotional intelligence and affectivity on emotional labor strategies at work, Journal of Individual Difference, 30(2), p75-86. [https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.30.2.75]
15. Choo, H., Kim, H., & Jun, D., (2010), The effect of a salesperson’s affectivity and the performance stressor on emotional labor at the department stores, Journal of the Korean Society of Clothing and Textiles, 34(3), p411-423. [https://doi.org/10.5850/JKSCT.2010.34.3.411]
16. Christensen, C., & Raynor, M., (2003), Why hard-nosed executives should care about management theory, Harvard Business Review, 81(9), p67-74.
17. Diefendorff, J. M., & Gosserand, R. H., (2003), Understanding the emotional labor process: A control theory perspective, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24(8), p945-959.
18. Ekman, P., (1972), Universal and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971, p207-283, Linclon: University of Nebraska Press.
19. Glomb, T. M., & Tews, M. J., (2004), Emotional labor: A conceptualization and scale development, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64(1), p1-23.
20. Godwyn, M., (2006), Using emotional labor to create and maintain relationships in service interactions, Symbolic Interaction, 29(4), p487-506.
21. Goffman, E., (1959), The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
22. Gosserand, R. H., & Diefendorff, J. M., (2005), Emotional display rules and emotional labor: The moderating role of commitment, Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6), p1256-1264.
23. Grandey, A. A., (2000), Emotion regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), p95-110.
24. Grandey, A. A., Dickter, D. N., & Sin, H., (2004), The customer is not always right: Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(3), p397-418.
25. Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., & Steiner, D. D., (2005), Must “service with a smile” be stressful? The moderating role of personal control for U.S. and French employees, Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(5), p893-904.
26. Grandey, A. A., Kern, J. H., & Frone, M. R., (2007), Verbal abuse from outsiders versus from insiders: Comparing frequency, impact on emotional exhaustion, and the role of emotional labor, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(1), p63-79.
27. Gross, J., (1998), Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), p224-237.
28. Groth, M., Hennig-Thurau, T., & Walsh, G., (2009), Customer reactions to emotional labor: The roles of employee acting strategies and customer detection accuracy, Academy of Management Journal, 52(5), p958-974.
29. Gutek, B. A., (1995), The dynamics of service: Reflections on the changing number of customer/ provider interaction, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
30. Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R., (1976), Motivation through the design of work, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(2), p250-279.
31. Hochschild, A. R., (1979), Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure, American Journal of Society, 85(3), p551-575.
32. Hochschild, A. R., (1983), The managed heart, Berkely, CA: University of California Press.
33. Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F., (2011), On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16(6), p361-389.
34. Johanson, M. M., & Woods, R. H., (2008), Recognizing the emotional element in service excellence, Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 49(3), p310-16.
35. Johnson, H. M., & Spector, P. E., (2007), Service with a smile: Do emotional intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process?, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(4), p319-333.
36. Lazarus, R. S., (1991), Emotion and adaptation, New York; Oxford University Press.
37. Lee, H. S., & Lim, J. H., (2009), Structural equation modeling with AMOS 16.0, Paju, Gyeonggido: Bobmunsa.
38. Lee, S. H., & Kim, G. H., (2012), The qualitative study on the emotional labor of fashion sales personnel -focused on fast fashion sales personnel-, Journal of the Korean Society of Clothing and Textiles, 36(5), p534-548.
39. Lewig, K. A., & Dollard, M. F., (2003), Emotional dissonance, emotional exhaustion and job satisfaction in call centre workers, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 12(4), p366-392.
40. Liu, Y., Perrewé, P., Hochwarter, W., & Kacmar, C., (2004), Dispositional antecedents and consequences of emotional labor at work, Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 10(4), p12-26.
41. Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E., (1981), The measurement of experienced burnout, Journal of Occupational Behavior, 2(2), p99-118.
42. Mastracci, S. H., Newman, M. A., & Guy, M. E., (2010), Emotional labor: Why to teach it and how to teach it, Journal of Public Affairs Education, 16(2), p123-141.
43. Mikolajczak, M., Menil, C., & Luminet, O., (2007), Explaining the protective effect of trait emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional labor processes, Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), p1107-1117.
44. Miller, K. I., Stiff, J. B., & Ellis, B. H., (1988), Communication and empathy as precursors to burnout among human service workers, Communication Monographs, 55(3), p250-265.
45. Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E., (2006), The work design questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design and the nature of work, Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6), p1321-1339.
46. Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C., (1996), The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor, Academy of Management Review, 21(4), p986-1010.
47. Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C., (1997), Managing emotions in the workplace, Journal of Managerial Issues, 9(3), p257-274.
48. Muliawan, A. D., Green, P. F., & Robb, D. A., (2009), The turnover intentions of information systems auditors, International Journal of Accounting Information Systems, 10(3), p117-136.
49. Nunnally, J. C., (1978), Psychometric theory (2nd ed.), New York: McGraw Hill.
50. Otieno, R., Harrow, C., & Lea-Greenwood, G., (2005), The unhappy shopper, a retail experience: Exploring fashion, fit and affordability, International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 33(4), p298-309.
51. Rafaeli, A., & Sutton, R. I., (1987), Expression of emotion as part of the work role, Academy of Management Review, 12(1), p23-37.
52. Silter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J., (2010), How rude! Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee outcomes, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15(4), p468-481.
53. Spencer, S., & Rupp, D. E., (2009), Angry, guilty, and conflicted: Injustice toward coworkers heightens emotional labor through cognitive and emotional mechanisms, American Psychological Association, 94(2), p429-444.
54. Sung, T. J., (2007), Statistic analysis with SPSS/AMOS: from descriptive analysis to structural equation model, Seoul: Hakjisa.
55. Tang, C., Seal, C. R., Naumann, S. E., & Miguel, K., (2013), Emotional labor: The role of employee acting strategies on customer emotional experience and subsequent buying decisions, International Review of Management and Marketing, 3(2), p50-57.
56. Tett, R. P., & Meyer, J. P., (1993), Job satisfaction, organizational commitment turnover intention, and turnover: Path analysis based on meta-analytic findings, Personnel Psychology, 46(2), p259-293.
57. Totterdell, P., & Holman, D., (2003), Emotion regulation in customer service roles: Testing a model of emotional labor, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(1), p55-73.
58. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A., (1988), Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), p1063-1070.
59. Watson, D., Pennebaker, J. W., & Folger, R., (1987), Beyond negative affectivity: Measuring stress and satisfaction in the workplace, Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 8(2), p141-158.
60. Wilson, N. L., & Holmvall, C. M., (2013), The development and validation of the incivility from customer scale, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 18(3), p310-326.
61. Wong, C., Wong, P., & Law, K. S., (2007), Evidence of the practical utility of Wong‘s emotional intelligence scale in Hong Kong and mainland China, Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 24(1), p43-60.
62. Zerbe, W. J., (2000), Emotional dissonance and employee well-being. In N. M. Ashkanasy, C. E. J. Härtel, & W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice, p189-214, Westport, CT: Quorum/Greenwood.

Appendix
List of Measures Used in the Study


Employee Affectivity (Adapted From Watson et al., 1988)
1. I am optimistic in everything (Reverse coding)
2. I am upset my current state.
3. I am annoyed in everything.
4. I feel abandoned alone.
5. I am afraid of my life.
Job Autonomy (Adapted From Morgeson and Humphrey, 2006)
1. My job allows me to make my own decision about how to schedule my work.
2. My job gives me a chance to use my personal initiative or judgment in carrying out the work.
3. My job allows me to plan how I do my work.
4. My job provides me with significant autonomy in making decisions.
5. My job allows me to make decisions about what methods I use to complete my work.
6. My job allows me to decide on my own how to go about doing my work.
Customer Incivility (Adapted From Wilson and Holmvall, 2013)
1. How often have customers continued to complain despite your efforts to assist
them? 2. How often have customers made gestures to express their impatience?
3. How often have customers grumbled to you about slow service during busy times?
4. How often have customers made negative remarks to you about your organization?
5. How often have customers blamed you for a problem you did not cause?
6. How often have customers used an inappropriate manner of addressing you?
7. How often have customers failed to acknowledge your efforts when you have gone out of your way to help them?
8. How often have customers grumbled to you that there were too fee employees working?
9. How often have customers complained to you about the value of goods and services?
10. How often have customers made inappropriate gestures to get your attention?
Emotional Labor (Adapted From Choo et al., 2010)
Surface Acting
1. I display emotions that I am not actually feeling.
2. When getting ready for work, I tell myself that I am going to have a good day.
3. I fake the emotions I show when dealing with customers.
4. I try to actually experience the emotions that I must show when interacting with customers.
5. I have to concentrate more on my behavior when I display an emotion that I don’t actually feel.
Deep Dcting
6. I actually feel the emotions that I need to show to do my job.
7. I put on a mask in order to express the right emotions for my job.
8. I work at calling up the feelfeelings I need to show to customers.
9. The emotions I show to customers match what I truly feel.
10. I have to cover up my true feelings when dealing with customers.
Self-efficacy (Adapted From Chen et al., 2001)
1. I will be able to achieve most of the goals that I have set for myself.
2. When facing difficult tasks, I am certain that I will accomplish them.
3. I believe I can succeed at most any endeavor to which I set my mind.
4. I am confident that I can perform effectively on many different tasks.
5. Compared to other people, I can do most tasks very well.
Turnover intention (Adapted From Muliawan et al., 2009)
1. In case I have the chance, I hope to move to another company.
2. I will looking for a chance to move to another company sooner or later.
3. I will look for another job in the future.
4. I would quit my job if my job gets worse than now.
Burnout (Adapted From Maslach and Jackson, 1981)
1. I feel emotionally drained from my work.
2. I feel used up at the end of the workday.
3. I feel fatigued when I get up in the morning.
4. Working with people all day is really a strain for me.
5. I feel burned out from my work.
6. I feel frustrated by my job.
7. I feel I’m working too hard on my job.
8. Working with people directly puts too much stress on me.
9. I feel like I’m at the end of my rope.


Biographical Note:Gihyung Kim (first author) is currently a Ph.D. candidate and a lecturer at the Sookmyung Women’s University (Seoul, Korea). She obtained AAS in Fashion Merchandising Management at the Fashion Institute of Technology (New York, USA), BA in Clothing and Textile at the Sookmyung Women’s University (Seoul, Korea), and MBA in Marketing Management at the Pace University (New York, USA). Her research interests include fashion marketing and merchandising, retail environment and business strategy. E-mail: claire-g-kim@sm.ac.kr

Biographical Note:Seunghee Lee (corresponding author) is an associate professor at the Sookmyung Women’s University (Seoul, Korea) in the subject of Clothing and Textile Department. She obtained her M.A. and Ph.D. degrees from Auburn University, USA. Her teaching and research interests include consumer behavior, body image, fashion marketing, and retailing. E-mail: leeseu1@sm.ac.kr


Keywords: Emotional labor, clothing service, employee affectivity, job autonomy, customer incivility.